Any regular leisure-time physical activity at any age is linked to better brain function in later life, suggests a study published online in the Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.
The findings also suggested that maintaining an exercise routine throughout adulthood seems to be best for preserving mental acuity and memory.
Even though factoring in childhood cognitive ability, household income and education weakened the observed associations, the findings remained statistically significant.
Physical activity is modestly associated with a lower risk of dementia, cognitive decline and loss of later life mental acuity.
But it’s not known whether the timing, frequency or maintenance of leisure-time physical activity across the life course might be key to later life cognitive abilities.
The researchers were particularly keen to know if physical activity might be most beneficial in specific “sensitive” periods across the life course, or across multiple time periods.
To try and find out, they looked at the strength of associations between a range of cognitive tests at age 69 and reported leisure-time physical activity at the ages of 36, 43, 53, 60-64, and 69 in 1,417 people (53% women) taking part in the 1946 British birth cohort study.
Physical activity levels were categorised as: inactive; moderately active (one to four times per month); most active (five or more times per month).
These levels were added together across all five age assessments to create a total score ranging from zero (inactive at all ages) to five (active at all ages).
Some 11% of participants were physically inactive at all five time points, 17% were active at one, 20% were active at two and three, 17% were active at four, and 15% at all five.
Cognitive performance at age 69 was assessed using the validated ACE-111, which tests attention and orientation, verbal fluency, memory, language, and visuospatial function.
The 69-year-olds were also given tests of verbal memory (word learning test) and processing speed (visual search speed).
Factors associated with a heightened risk of cognitive decline – cardiovascular (heart) and mental health, and carriage of the APOE-4 gene – were also assessed to see if these modified any observed associations.
Analysis of the results showed that being physically active at all five time points was associated with higher cognitive performance, verbal memory and processing speed at the age of 69.
The effect sizes were similar across all adult ages and for those who were moderately and most physically active, “suggesting that being physically active at any time in adulthood, even if participating as little as once per month, is linked with higher cognition,” write the researchers.
But the strongest association was observed for sustained cumulative physical activity and later life cognition, and for those who were most physically active at all ages.
The positive association between cumulative physical activity and later life cognitive performance was partly explained by childhood cognition, socioeconomic position and education.
But the effect remained significant when these were factored in, and the associations weren’t explained by differences in later life cardiovascular or mental health.
“Together, these results suggest that the initiation and maintenance of physical activity across adulthood may be more important than the timing... or the frequency of physical activity at a specific period,” say the researchers.
This is an observational study, and as such, can’t establish cause, and the researchers acknowledge various limitations to their findings.
The study included only White participants and had a disproportionately high attrition rate among those who were socially disadvantaged.
No information was available on exercise intensity, duration or adherence either.
But the researchers nevertheless conclude: “Our findings support guidelines to recommend participation in any physical activity across adulthood and provide evidence that encouraging inactive adults to be more active at any time and encouraging already active adults to maintain activity, could confer benefits on later life cognition.”